Skip to main content

Herbert Terrace

  • Living reference work entry
  • First Online:
Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior
  • 96 Accesses

Herbert S. Terrace is a professor of psychology at Columbia University. He is best known for his experimental work in comparative and animal cognition, particularly on the topics of errorless learning, serial learning, and metacognition, as well as for attempting to train a chimpanzee to use sign language, and for his analysis of the evolution of language.

Early Life and Education

Terrace was born November 29, 1936. He received an undergraduate degree in psychology from Cornell University in 1957 and a PhD from Harvard University in 1961, advised by B. F. Skinner. Among his early influences, Terrace credits Skinner’s approach to operant conditioning as influencing him substantially. However, two other figures (S. S. Stevens and H. M. Jenkins) also shaped his early views (Terrace 2010b), motivating his initial research interest in dissecting the role of psychophysical discriminations in operant behaviors. Work in this domain gradually revealed the limitations of a purely behavioral...

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Avdagic, E., Jensen, G., Altschul, D., & Terrace, H. S. (2014). Rapid cognitive flexibility of rhesus macaques performing psychophysical task-switching. Animal Cognition, 17, 619–631.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brannon, E. M., & Terrace, H. S. (1998). Ordering of the numerosities 1 to 9 by monkeys. Science, 282, 746–749.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, S., Swartz, K. B., & Terrace, H. S. (1997). Knowledge of the ordinal position of list items in rhesus monkeys. Psychological Science, 8, 80–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibbon, J., Baldock, M. D., Locurto, C., Gold, L., & Terrace, H. S. (1977). Trial and intertrial durations in autoshaping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 3, 264–284.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, G., Altschul, D., Danly, E., & Terrace, H. S. (2013). Transfer of a serial representation between two distinct tasks by rhesus macaques. PloS One, 8, e70285.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, G., Muñoz, F., Alkan, Y., Ferrera, V. P., & Terrace, H. S. (2015). Implicit value updating explains transitive inference performance: The betasort model. PLoS Computational Biology, 11, e1004523.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kornell, N., Son, L. K., & Terrace, H. S. (2007). Transfer of metacognitive skills and hint seeking in monkeys. Psychological Science, 18, 64–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Merritt, D. J., & Terrace, H. S. (2011). Mechanisms of inferential order judgments in humans (Homo sapiens) and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 125, 227–238.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J., & Terrace, H. S. (Eds.). (2013). Agency and joint attention. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, G., Kornell, N., Kornblum, T., & Terrace, H. S. (2014). Retrospective and prospective metacognitive judgments in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Animal Cognition, 17, 249–257.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G., & Terrace, H. S. (Eds.). (1984). Animal cognition. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Terrace, H. S. (2017). In the beginning. Journal of Language Evolution, 2, 114–125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Subiaul, F., Romansky, K., Cantlon, J. F., Klein, T., & Terrace, H. S. (2007). Cognitive imitation in 2-year-old children (Homo sapiens): A comparison with rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 6, 1–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanner, N., Jensen, G., Ferrera, V. P., & Terrace, H. S. (2017). Inferential learning of serial order of perceptual categories by rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Journal of Neuroscience. 37, 6268–6276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S. (1964). Wavelength generalization after discrimination learning with and without errors. Science, 144, 78–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S. (1972). By-products of discrimination learning. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), Psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 5, pp. 195–265). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S. (1979). Nim: A chimpanzee who learned sign language. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S. (1984a). Animal cognition. In H. L. Roitblat, T. G. Bever, & H. S. Terrace (Eds.), Animal cognition (pp. 7–28). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S. (1984b). Simultaneous chaining: The problem it poses for traditional chaining theory. In M. L. Commons, R. J. Herrnstein, & A. R. Wagner (Eds.), Quantitative analyses of behavior: Discrimination processes (pp. 115–138). Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S. (1985). In the beginning was the “name”. American Psychologist, 40, 1011–1028.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S. (1987). Chunking by a pigeon in a serial learning task. Nature, 325, 149–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S. (2010a). The comparative psychology of serially organized behavior. Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews, 5, 23–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S. (2010b). Defining the stimulus – A memoir. Behavioural Processes, 83, 139–153.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S. (2011). Missing links in the evolution of language. In S. Dehaene & Y. Christen (Eds.), Characterizing consciousness: From cognition to clinic (pp. 1–25). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S. (forthcoming). Becoming human: Why two minds are better than one. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S., & Bever, T. G. (1976). What might be learned from studying language in chimpanzees? The importance of symbolizing oneself. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 280, 579–588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S., & Metcalfe, J. (Eds.). (2005). The missing link in cognition: Origins of self-reflective consciousness. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S., & Son, L. K. (2009). Comparative metacognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 19, 67–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S., Petitto, L. A., Sanders, R. J., & Bever, T. G. (1979). Can an ape create a sentence? Science, 206, 891–902.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Terrace, H. S., Son, L. K., & Brannon, E. M. (2003). Serial expertise of rhesus macaques. Psychological Science, 14, 66–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Greg Jensen .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Section Editor information

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer International Publishing AG

About this entry

Cite this entry

Jensen, G. (2017). Herbert Terrace. In: Vonk, J., Shackelford, T. (eds) Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_927-1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_927-1

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Cham

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-319-47829-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-319-47829-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Reference Behavioral Science and PsychologyReference Module Humanities and Social SciencesReference Module Business, Economics and Social Sciences

Publish with us

Policies and ethics